MEASURING OCCUPANCY
mean value of k is 2.69, and the mean value of k/n is 0.039. Also shown are the predicted values of k corresponding to the maximum- likelihood estimate of the distribution of p, which itself is given in Figure 5B. The mean of this distribution is 0.021, distinctly lower than k=n. This mean tacitly incorporates the occu- pancy probability of all genera, including unsampled genera, assuming they are all drawn from the same underlying distribution. In addition to estimating the occupancy probability distribution of all taxa, we can estimate the part of the distribution corre- sponding to subsets of taxa with specified values of k. Those with k>0 preferentially sample higher values of p than those with k=0 (Fig. 5C,D), and those with progressively higher values of k reflect ever higher values of p (Fig. 5D). Even though the taxa with k>0 represent a biased subset of the entire prob- ability distribution (Fig. 5C), fitting that dis- tribution allows this bias to be circumvented. Truncating the log-normal at pmax is admittedly ad hoc and inelegant. John Alroy (personal communication 2016) has kindly suggested an alternative approach: to model the probabilities as a logit-normal distribution, in which logit(p), equal to log[p/(1 − p)], is normally distributed (Coull and Agresti 1999; Dorazio et al. 2006: p. 847). With p bounded between 0 and 1, logit(p) is unbounded from –∞to∞, no ad hoc truncation is necessary, and the distribution can be modeled with only two parameters, μlogit (the mean of the logit of p) and σlogit (the standard deviation of the logit of p). With the low values of p characteristic of the data analyzed here, the log-normal and logit- normal are rather similar. Moreover, by the time the truncation point is reached, the probability density has generally trailed off nearly to zero, so the fitted log-normal prob- ability curve does not “drop off a cliff” at pmax. The case in Figure 5 is typical. If for each stage we compare the probability density at pmax to the maximum value of the density, the ratio of the two has a median ~1×10−4, and it is never higher than ~1×10−3. The median tail area to the right of pmax is ~0.3%, and the maximum is ~2%. Figure 6 shows results of fitting three different distributions to each stage: (1) constant p;
713
(2) truncated log-normal p; and (3) logit- normal p. Figure 6A compares the predicted and observed standard deviation of k.Ifwe assume p is constant for each stage and use the Haldane–Fisher approach to estimate it, the predicted variance in k is substantially lower than what is observed. If we fit a logit-normal, the predicted variance is too high. This is because most genera have observed occupancy values, k, far to the left of the theoretical maximum value of n, that is, their observed proportional occupancies, k/n, are far below 1.0. However, the logit-normal integrates all the way to p= 1.0 (i.e., logit[p]=∞) and there- fore predicts some high values of k that increase the expected variance. (Similar results [not shown] obtain if we fit a log-normal truncated at p=1.0.) If instead we fita truncated log-normal, we accurately capture the observed variation in k. Calculating the sample size–corrected AIC for each model for each stage (Fig. 6B), we see that the Akaike weights generally favor the truncated log- normal (median weight 0.80 vs. 0.20 for the logit-normal and 0.0 for the constant-p model) and that the median truncation point is around 0.25 (Fig. 6C). The agreement between predicted and
observed variance in k is one sign that the truncated log-normal gives a reasonable fitto the data. Another sign is that the dependence between the number of cells, n, and the estimate of mean occupancy probability, p, essentially vanishes with this model (Spearman rank-order correlation coefficient: rs= −0.13, p > 0.25), whereas some bias, albeit weak, persists if we fit the logit-normal (rs= −0.35, p<0.01) (Fig. 7). The lack of correlation between n and p is to be expected if the data conform to the modeled form of the distribution, because n is explicitly taken into account in the likelihood function (Appendix). Whether to prefer the logit-normal or the
truncated log-normal is somewhat a question of truth versus beauty. For aesthetics the logit- normal takes the prize, but given the data considered here, the truncated log-normal generally fits better, enough so to justify the extra parameter. I will therefore stick with the truncated log-normal for the rest of this paper. However, there is no obvious reason to
Page 1 |
Page 2 |
Page 3 |
Page 4 |
Page 5 |
Page 6 |
Page 7 |
Page 8 |
Page 9 |
Page 10 |
Page 11 |
Page 12 |
Page 13 |
Page 14 |
Page 15 |
Page 16 |
Page 17 |
Page 18 |
Page 19 |
Page 20 |
Page 21 |
Page 22 |
Page 23 |
Page 24 |
Page 25 |
Page 26 |
Page 27 |
Page 28 |
Page 29 |
Page 30 |
Page 31 |
Page 32 |
Page 33 |
Page 34 |
Page 35 |
Page 36 |
Page 37 |
Page 38 |
Page 39 |
Page 40 |
Page 41 |
Page 42 |
Page 43 |
Page 44 |
Page 45 |
Page 46 |
Page 47 |
Page 48 |
Page 49 |
Page 50 |
Page 51 |
Page 52 |
Page 53 |
Page 54 |
Page 55 |
Page 56 |
Page 57 |
Page 58 |
Page 59 |
Page 60 |
Page 61 |
Page 62 |
Page 63 |
Page 64 |
Page 65 |
Page 66 |
Page 67 |
Page 68 |
Page 69 |
Page 70 |
Page 71 |
Page 72 |
Page 73 |
Page 74 |
Page 75 |
Page 76 |
Page 77 |
Page 78 |
Page 79 |
Page 80 |
Page 81 |
Page 82 |
Page 83 |
Page 84 |
Page 85 |
Page 86 |
Page 87 |
Page 88 |
Page 89 |
Page 90 |
Page 91 |
Page 92 |
Page 93 |
Page 94 |
Page 95 |
Page 96 |
Page 97 |
Page 98 |
Page 99 |
Page 100 |
Page 101 |
Page 102 |
Page 103 |
Page 104 |
Page 105 |
Page 106 |
Page 107 |
Page 108 |
Page 109 |
Page 110 |
Page 111 |
Page 112 |
Page 113 |
Page 114 |
Page 115 |
Page 116 |
Page 117 |
Page 118 |
Page 119 |
Page 120 |
Page 121 |
Page 122 |
Page 123 |
Page 124 |
Page 125 |
Page 126 |
Page 127 |
Page 128 |
Page 129 |
Page 130 |
Page 131 |
Page 132 |
Page 133 |
Page 134 |
Page 135 |
Page 136 |
Page 137 |
Page 138 |
Page 139 |
Page 140 |
Page 141 |
Page 142 |
Page 143 |
Page 144 |
Page 145 |
Page 146 |
Page 147 |
Page 148 |
Page 149 |
Page 150 |
Page 151 |
Page 152 |
Page 153 |
Page 154 |
Page 155 |
Page 156 |
Page 157 |
Page 158 |
Page 159 |
Page 160 |
Page 161 |
Page 162 |
Page 163 |
Page 164 |
Page 165 |
Page 166 |
Page 167 |
Page 168 |
Page 169 |
Page 170 |
Page 171 |
Page 172 |
Page 173 |
Page 174 |
Page 175 |
Page 176 |
Page 177 |
Page 178 |
Page 179 |
Page 180 |
Page 181 |
Page 182 |
Page 183 |
Page 184 |
Page 185 |
Page 186 |
Page 187 |
Page 188 |
Page 189 |
Page 190 |
Page 191 |
Page 192