6 VYGOTSKY AND PIAGET: THOUGHT AND LANGUAGE
Vygotsky and Piaget: thought and language human canone from
situation time
development sense
apes thought
language intellectual
action tools talk
Both Vygotsky and Piaget made significant contributions to research into the development of thought and language. Although their models differed in several ways, their effect on teaching methods has been, in some respects, similar. This article will outline their approaches to cognitive development, highlight the main differences between their theories, and illustrate their influence on modern classroom practice.
Vygotsky’s theory of cognitive development is based on the social constructivist approach, which proposes that both cognitive and linguistic skills are developed through social interaction. Language is an interface between people, allowing them to construct and negotiate meaning. Importantly, Vygotsky describes language as a cultural tool, a means of passing on the accumulated knowledge of a society from generation to generation.
According to Vygotsky, cognitive development is culturally determined, which means that children in some cultures will develop certain cognitive skills to a higher level than in others, depending on the importance of that particular skill to the culture. For example, children who are brought up in a society that values technology will learn to interpret icons on electronic equipment at a comparatively young age. In contrast, children in societies or domains such as the indigenous people of Australia, that value the ability to navigate a landscape by its geographical features, will develop the cognitive skills crucial to this particular ability. In other words, learning is domain dependent.
Vygotsky also suggested that cognitive development was dependent on the social interaction of the child with an experienced ‘mentor’, who would lead it from its actual stage of development to the next. Vygotsky used the term ‘zone of proximal development’ to describe a situation in which a child receives support and guidance, known as ‘scaffolding’, to master a new skill. Vygotsky considered language to be an essential element in this process.
In contrast to this approach, Piaget’s model is based on an idealized child living in a social vacuum. He proposed that all children acquire cognitive skills at predetermined stages and in a specific order,
regardless of the context in which they are learning. Piaget’s model of cognitive development, like Vygotsky’s, assumes that children will develop mental structures, or schemata, by interacting experimentally with their physical environment. To put it simply, Piaget saw the child as a ‘scientist’, continually testing theories and learning from the results. According to Piaget, when actual knowledge, a current schema, is contradicted by new information, a state of ‘disequilibrium’ is created, and the enquiring mind tries to restore the balance by adapting the schema to accommodate the new information. Unlike Vygotsky, however, Piaget’s paradigm largely ignores the importance of social interaction on the cognitive development of the child and, consequently, the role of language in this process.
It is important to recognize that both Piaget and Vygotsky have made major contributions to modern teaching methods. For instance, Piaget’s focus on the stages of cognitive development, and Vygotsky’s concept of scaffolding in the zone of proximal development have resulted in carefully structured curricula for pre-school and primary education, ensuring that children are given realistic learning goals and adequate support. Just as significantly, Vygotsky’s emphasis on the importance of social interaction in language acquisition has had a profound effect on language teaching methods for both children and adults. A final example of the influence of both scientists on teaching approaches is the relatively recent development of peer teaching and collaborative learning methods. Piaget’s theory suggests that listening to the opinions of other learners increases instances of disequilibrium and, consequently, learning. Vygotsky’s model, on the other hand, encourages more advanced learners to teach less experienced learners through social interaction and language.
49
stage speak vocal like
change
use
voice separate analogy
chimpanzee
speech social
think school
child behaviour
Page 1 |
Page 2 |
Page 3 |
Page 4 |
Page 5 |
Page 6 |
Page 7 |
Page 8 |
Page 9 |
Page 10 |
Page 11 |
Page 12 |
Page 13 |
Page 14 |
Page 15 |
Page 16 |
Page 17 |
Page 18 |
Page 19 |
Page 20 |
Page 21 |
Page 22 |
Page 23 |
Page 24 |
Page 25 |
Page 26 |
Page 27 |
Page 28 |
Page 29 |
Page 30 |
Page 31 |
Page 32 |
Page 33 |
Page 34 |
Page 35 |
Page 36 |
Page 37 |
Page 38 |
Page 39 |
Page 40 |
Page 41 |
Page 42 |
Page 43 |
Page 44 |
Page 45 |
Page 46 |
Page 47 |
Page 48 |
Page 49 |
Page 50 |
Page 51 |
Page 52 |
Page 53 |
Page 54 |
Page 55 |
Page 56 |
Page 57 |
Page 58 |
Page 59 |
Page 60 |
Page 61 |
Page 62 |
Page 63 |
Page 64 |
Page 65 |
Page 66 |
Page 67 |
Page 68 |
Page 69 |
Page 70 |
Page 71 |
Page 72 |
Page 73 |
Page 74 |
Page 75 |
Page 76 |
Page 77 |
Page 78 |
Page 79 |
Page 80 |
Page 81 |
Page 82 |
Page 83 |
Page 84 |
Page 85 |
Page 86 |
Page 87 |
Page 88 |
Page 89 |
Page 90 |
Page 91 |
Page 92 |
Page 93 |
Page 94 |
Page 95 |
Page 96 |
Page 97 |
Page 98 |
Page 99 |
Page 100 |
Page 101 |
Page 102 |
Page 103 |
Page 104 |
Page 105 |
Page 106 |
Page 107 |
Page 108 |
Page 109 |
Page 110 |
Page 111 |
Page 112 |
Page 113 |
Page 114 |
Page 115 |
Page 116 |
Page 117 |
Page 118 |
Page 119 |
Page 120 |
Page 121 |
Page 122 |
Page 123 |
Page 124 |
Page 125 |
Page 126 |
Page 127 |
Page 128 |
Page 129 |
Page 130 |
Page 131 |
Page 132 |
Page 133 |
Page 134 |
Page 135 |
Page 136 |
Page 137