TRANSCRIPTS
word long; for example, Mama, Dada, milk, cat. But by their second birthday, children are beginning to use two-word utterances. And between two and three years old, children are speaking in sentences. Children’s vocabulary develops rapidly at this stage, and the average five-year-old will have a vocabulary of between 10,000 and 15,000 words. I’d like to remind you at this point that all the ages we’ve been looking at are approximate. Individual children develop at different speeds.
So far, we’ve been looking at the development of
speaking and vocabulary. But we must remember that children’s language development is restricted to the concepts they are capable of understanding. So now, I’d like to examine the connection between language acquisition and cognitive development.
The psychologist, Piaget … that’s P-I-A-G-E-T,
identified three stages of cognitive development in children: preoperational, concrete operations and formal operations.
According to Piaget, children between two and
seven or eight are at the preoperational stage – when they have difficulty grasping abstract concepts, like time, and the difference between fantasy and reality. Their language development reflects this.
However, between the ages of seven and eleven,
children enter the period of concrete operations – when they learn how to understand and express abstract ideas based on concrete objects, for example, drawing maps and telling the time.
And, finally, at the age of about eleven or twelve,
in the period of formal operations, children’s language has developed to match their ability to argue logically using abstract ideas.
Unit 1, Lesson 1.4, Exercise E≤1.9
Lecture 4 Good morning. Everybody ready? Good. Now, today I’ll be looking at the historical origins of psychology and some of the key developments that have led to its current scientific status. The scientific approach to the study of the mind is based on empiricism; the theory that we can only know things through physical and observable evidence, and you will see that a number of early theories have been validated by modern experimental research.
Let’s start in 435 BC, when Alcmaeon … that’s
spelt, A-L-C-M-A-E-O-N, who was born in Croton, in Southern Italy, experimented with anatomical dissection and discovered the optic nerve.
Alcmaeon theorized that the brain was connected to the rest of the body through currents of energy. This is a surprisingly modern description of the nervous system.
Now moving on five hundred years to 129 AD, we
come to Galen, who was a physician, born in Asia Minor (modern Turkey). Galen’s studies of anatomy led him to identify the cranial nerves, in the brain, and the cerebro-spinal fluid, which irrigates the brain and the nervous system. This is another early discovery that’s still valid today.
For well over a thousand years after Galen’s
discoveries, there was very little scientific investigation into the structure of the brain. However, in the 16th
century, the Belgian anatomist,
Andreas Vesalius (that’s V-E-S-A-L-I-U-S), who lived between 1514 and 1564, continued Galen’s work and wrote a complete textbook about human anatomy based on his dissections of the body.
A hundred and fifty years later, in 1690, the
British empiricist, John Locke, argued that our perception of the world depends entirely on our sensory experiences; but he added that we also have a mental faculty that allows us to reflect on what we experience. This theory was supported later by the work of the psychologist, Wundt, as we’ll see in a minute.
Let’s continue now to look at the physical study
of the brain. The German physiologist, Franz Joseph Gall, who lived from 1758–1828, believed that different parts of the brain were responsible for producing particular behaviours. His theory that the shape of the skull reflected the personality of an individual has been disproved; but his fundamental assumption that certain areas of the brain have specific functions is now supported by modern neuropsychology.
I’ll conclude by mentioning Wilhelm Wundt,
who founded the first laboratory dedicated to psychological research in 1879. Wundt is important in the history of psychology because, in addition to researching the physical effects of stimuli on behaviour, he asked the subjects of his experiments to reflect on and report their experiences. This combination of scientific experimentation and introspection was a forerunner to modern approaches to psychotherapy.
Unit 1, Lesson 1.4, Exercise E≤1.10
Lecture 5 In this morning’s lecture, I’m going to compare two types of conditioning, also known as
115
Page 1 |
Page 2 |
Page 3 |
Page 4 |
Page 5 |
Page 6 |
Page 7 |
Page 8 |
Page 9 |
Page 10 |
Page 11 |
Page 12 |
Page 13 |
Page 14 |
Page 15 |
Page 16 |
Page 17 |
Page 18 |
Page 19 |
Page 20 |
Page 21 |
Page 22 |
Page 23 |
Page 24 |
Page 25 |
Page 26 |
Page 27 |
Page 28 |
Page 29 |
Page 30 |
Page 31 |
Page 32 |
Page 33 |
Page 34 |
Page 35 |
Page 36 |
Page 37 |
Page 38 |
Page 39 |
Page 40 |
Page 41 |
Page 42 |
Page 43 |
Page 44 |
Page 45 |
Page 46 |
Page 47 |
Page 48 |
Page 49 |
Page 50 |
Page 51 |
Page 52 |
Page 53 |
Page 54 |
Page 55 |
Page 56 |
Page 57 |
Page 58 |
Page 59 |
Page 60 |
Page 61 |
Page 62 |
Page 63 |
Page 64 |
Page 65 |
Page 66 |
Page 67 |
Page 68 |
Page 69 |
Page 70 |
Page 71 |
Page 72 |
Page 73 |
Page 74 |
Page 75 |
Page 76 |
Page 77 |
Page 78 |
Page 79 |
Page 80 |
Page 81 |
Page 82 |
Page 83 |
Page 84 |
Page 85 |
Page 86 |
Page 87 |
Page 88 |
Page 89 |
Page 90 |
Page 91 |
Page 92 |
Page 93 |
Page 94 |
Page 95 |
Page 96 |
Page 97 |
Page 98 |
Page 99 |
Page 100 |
Page 101 |
Page 102 |
Page 103 |
Page 104 |
Page 105 |
Page 106 |
Page 107 |
Page 108 |
Page 109 |
Page 110 |
Page 111 |
Page 112 |
Page 113 |
Page 114 |
Page 115 |
Page 116 |
Page 117 |
Page 118 |
Page 119 |
Page 120 |
Page 121 |
Page 122 |
Page 123 |
Page 124 |
Page 125 |
Page 126 |
Page 127 |
Page 128 |
Page 129 |
Page 130 |
Page 131 |
Page 132 |
Page 133 |
Page 134 |
Page 135 |
Page 136 |
Page 137