search.noResults

search.searching

saml.title
dataCollection.invalidEmail
note.createNoteMessage

search.noResults

search.searching

orderForm.title

orderForm.productCode
orderForm.description
orderForm.quantity
orderForm.itemPrice
orderForm.price
orderForm.totalPrice
orderForm.deliveryDetails.billingAddress
orderForm.deliveryDetails.deliveryAddress
orderForm.noItems
Human–elephant interactions in Gabon 267 Only five interviewees proposed a halt to logging as a


resolution to human–elephant conflict, despite 34 intervie- wees stating that logging is a driver of the conflict. Perhaps people perceive the economic benefits of logging to out- weigh the costs of human–elephant conflict, perceive stop- ping logging as politically impossible or perceive it to be too late to mitigate the damage done by logging. For example, large moabi Baillonella toxisperma trees, a species cate- gorized as Vulnerable on the IUCN Red List (White, 1998) because of overexploitation, the fruits of which are eaten by elephants, have disappeared from logged forests near villages. Changes in forest ecology beyond logging were not mentioned by survey respondents but are also occur- ring: in nearby Lopé National Park there was an 81%decline in tree fruiting during 1986–2018, associated with an 11% decline in forest elephant body condition during 2008– 2018 (Bush et al., 2020). The spatial and temporal dynamics of ivory poaching and how elephants respond to this have also undergone change. Studies comparing levels of human–elephant interaction in villages with logged and unlogged surrounding forests need to be conducted to examine the relationship between logging and human– elephant conflict. Of the seven survey respondents who perceived conser-


vation policy as a driver of human–elephant conflict, four came from a village on the banks of the Ivindo river, close to the town of Makokou. This village is the closest of our study villages to Ivindo National Park headquarters and, of the villages in our study, has the most frequent interaction with park personnel. The section of the National Park close to this village is also contiguous with the forest around other villages along the national highway. It is thus possible that the National Park may not have differing effects on con- flict across these villages but rather that local perceptions of the park–human–elephant interaction vary. This would contradict findings from a study in northern Congo that showed perceptions of elephants to be most positive in the village with the strongest conservation presence (Nsonsi et al., 2018). Both human–elephant interactions and solu- tions to any conflict can have effects on how conservation efforts in general are perceived and thus responded to, and effectively reducing negative human–wildlife interac- tions remains a key challenge for the conservation not only of forest elephants but of the wider biodiversity of the Congo Basin rainforest. The purpose of our study was to determine when and


where perceptions of negative wildlife interactions corre- spond with actual visitation events and which factors (e.g. crop availability) might drive these perceptions. We identi- fied that the degree of agreement between perceptions and actual human–wildlife interactions depended on the iden- tity of the elephant and its movement patterns. This could help future studies to determine the behavioural profiles of the elephants responsible for crop damage and to make


more effective decisions about crop protection and elephant conservation in Gabon.


Acknowledgements We thank Nicholas School of the Environment of Duke University for financial and logistical support in preparing the study and in administering field surveys in the villages aroundMakokou; the village chiefs and rural peoplewho encouraged the publication of this study; the Institut de Recherche en Ecologie Tropicale within the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique etTechnologique for permission to conduct research in Gabon; the Agence National des Parcs Nationaux and Duke University for permission to use the elephant collar data; Carole Mbamy for her help with fieldwork; and the Navjot Sodhi Conservation Research Award for their recognition of this work.


Author contributions Study design: all authors; fieldwork: WM; data analysis: WM, CB, GZLF; writing: WM, CB, GZLF, JRP.


Conflicts of interest None.


Ethical standards This research abided by the Oryx guidelines on ethical standards. We obtained authorization to conduct research in Gabon through the Institut de Recherche en Ecologie Tropicale within the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique et Technologique (N\degree AR0057/18/MESRS/CENAREST/CG/CST/CSAR and N\de- gree AR004/20/MESRTT/CENAREST/CG/CST/CSAR). We followed the principle of free, prior and informed consent at both the community and individual levels.Communities gave oral prior and informed consent during meetings before we conducted any research; individual respon- dents gave oral consent in response to awritten prior and informed con- sent statement that we read to them. Respondents understood that we would publish anonymized data and explicitly asked us to name their communities in all research publications.


References


BASTILLE-ROUSSEAU,G. &WITTEMYER,G.(2019) Leveraging multidimensional heterogeneity in resource selection to define movement tactics of animals. Ecology Letters, 22, 1417–1427.


BEIRNE, C., HOUSLAY, T.M., MORKEL, P., CLARK, C.J., FAY, M., OKOUYI, J. et al. (2021) African forest elephant movements depend on time scale and individual behavior. Scientific Reports, 11, 12634.


BEIRNE, C., MEIER, A.C., BRUMAGIN, G., JASPERSE-SJOLANDER, L., LEWIS, M., MASSELOUX, J. et al. (2020) Climatic and resource determinants of forest elephant movements. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 8, 96.


BEIRNE, C.,MEIER, A.C.,MBELE, A.E.,MENIE MENIE, G., FROESE, G., OKOUYI,J.&POULSEN, J.R. (2019) Participatory monitoring reveals village-centered gradients of mammalian defaunation in Central Africa. Biological Conservation, 233, 228–238.


BENNETT, N.J. (2016) Using perceptions as evidence to improve conservation and environmental management: perceptions and conservation. Conservation Biology, 30, 582–592.


BLAKE, S., DEEM, S.L., STRINDBERG, S., MAISELS, F., MOMONT, L., ISIA, I.-B. et al. (2008) Roadless wilderness area determines forest elephant movements in the Congo Basin. PLOS One, 3,e3546.


BONNIER, A., FINNÉ,M. &WEIBERG,E.(2019) Examining land-use through GIS-based kernel density estimation: a re-evaluation of legacy data from the Berbati-Limnes survey. Journal of Field Archaeology, 44, 70–83.


BUCHHOLTZ, E.K., FITZGERALD, L.A., SONGHURST, A.,MCCULLOCH, G.P.&STRONZA, A.L. (2020) Experts and elephants: local ecological knowledge predicts landscape use for a species involved in human– wildlife conflict. Ecology and Society, 25, 26.


Oryx, 2024, 58(2), 261–268 © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S0030605323000704


Page 1  |  Page 2  |  Page 3  |  Page 4  |  Page 5  |  Page 6  |  Page 7  |  Page 8  |  Page 9  |  Page 10  |  Page 11  |  Page 12  |  Page 13  |  Page 14  |  Page 15  |  Page 16  |  Page 17  |  Page 18  |  Page 19  |  Page 20  |  Page 21  |  Page 22  |  Page 23  |  Page 24  |  Page 25  |  Page 26  |  Page 27  |  Page 28  |  Page 29  |  Page 30  |  Page 31  |  Page 32  |  Page 33  |  Page 34  |  Page 35  |  Page 36  |  Page 37  |  Page 38  |  Page 39  |  Page 40  |  Page 41  |  Page 42  |  Page 43  |  Page 44  |  Page 45  |  Page 46  |  Page 47  |  Page 48  |  Page 49  |  Page 50  |  Page 51  |  Page 52  |  Page 53  |  Page 54  |  Page 55  |  Page 56  |  Page 57  |  Page 58  |  Page 59  |  Page 60  |  Page 61  |  Page 62  |  Page 63  |  Page 64  |  Page 65  |  Page 66  |  Page 67  |  Page 68  |  Page 69  |  Page 70  |  Page 71  |  Page 72  |  Page 73  |  Page 74  |  Page 75  |  Page 76  |  Page 77  |  Page 78  |  Page 79  |  Page 80  |  Page 81  |  Page 82  |  Page 83  |  Page 84  |  Page 85  |  Page 86  |  Page 87  |  Page 88  |  Page 89  |  Page 90  |  Page 91  |  Page 92  |  Page 93  |  Page 94  |  Page 95  |  Page 96  |  Page 97  |  Page 98  |  Page 99  |  Page 100  |  Page 101  |  Page 102  |  Page 103  |  Page 104  |  Page 105  |  Page 106  |  Page 107  |  Page 108  |  Page 109  |  Page 110  |  Page 111  |  Page 112  |  Page 113  |  Page 114  |  Page 115  |  Page 116  |  Page 117  |  Page 118  |  Page 119  |  Page 120  |  Page 121  |  Page 122  |  Page 123  |  Page 124  |  Page 125  |  Page 126  |  Page 127  |  Page 128  |  Page 129  |  Page 130  |  Page 131  |  Page 132  |  Page 133  |  Page 134  |  Page 135  |  Page 136  |  Page 137  |  Page 138  |  Page 139  |  Page 140