This page contains a Flash digital edition of a book.
Trans RINA, Vol 155, Part C1, Intl J Marine Design, Jan -Jun 2013


What we do know is that dependencies arise between the activities of different


team members, creating the


requirement that those activities are coordinated in an orderly way. Examples from aviation show that in cockpit teams, captains make more errors than officers, and the “pilot flying” makes more errors than the “pilot not flying” (Jentsch, Barnett, Bowers, & Salas, 1999). This indicates both a lack


of joint situation awareness, an uneven


workload, as well as a lack of coordination in combination with of unclear roles and unclear monitoring responsibilities within the team. Similar teams work on the ship’s bridge and it has been observed that the role and responsibility division between team members is dynamic and changing (M Lützhöft, 2004). We should widen our perspective and regard the engine room crew as part of the team, both on a planning, operational and follow-up basis.


In order to understand the demands and requirements made by both departments (that is, the engine room and the bridge), it is essential to establish common knowledge and an understanding of each other’s working procedures. The engine department is to be informed whenever the situation demands a manned engine control room—for instance, in narrow passages or when approaching or leaving harbour. It is also necessary to increase the awareness of the current and future situation for the engine room crew. They can benefit greatly from knowing what is happening on the bridge and from being able to track the ship’s position. Unpublished field notes from a user-centred design project (Petersen, 2010) show that engine and bridge crew tasks work on the same timescale but often asynchronous.


Current sizes of displays used in modern ships are


somewhat smaller than the mechanical/physical solutions they replace: Engine room mimic-panels, as compared to current computer displays, are one example. Another is the size available for the display of electronic sea charts: The mandatory area is limited to 270 mm x 270 mm – whereas a typical sea chart is 10 times larger. There is empirical evidence suggesting that overview is reduced or even lost as a result (Hénique, Lindegaard, & Hunt, 2009). With electronic sea charts becoming mandatory in ships by 2012, this is a cause for concern, especially when it comes to the establishment of common understanding and team work in the bridge team. In the engine room, the situation is similar; analogous to other control room environments ashore, the large mimics (images of the layout of the system, usually covering a whole wall) have been removed and reduced to representations in individual computer screens. Overview and the possibility to get a joint image have been all but lost. One of these aspects is called the keyhole effect, which arises when the size of the virtual information space is larger than the available viewport of the CRT, allowing users to see only a small piece of the information space at one time (Woods, 1995). Other issues common in this transition are getting lost, thrashing (flipping between displays to see two or more pieces of information at “the same time”, increased mental workload from interface management and underutilization of functionality (Watts- Perotti & Woods, 1999). The latter is especially important


New technology has been added in an effort to increase maritime


technology.


safety, both general IT and communication Rapidly


advancing technological


developments, and the introduction of computers in the control rooms on board has altered traditional work tasks, transformed ergonomic requirements, as well as increased administrative demands (Knudsen, 2009; M. Lützhöft, Ljung, & Nodin, 2008; Wagner, Lundh, & Grundevik, 2008). Several studies show that many seafarers find new technology challenging (Allen, 2009; Lundh, 2010; M Lützhöft, 2004). In addition to engine room automation, changes in communication systems have also contributed to reduced crew manning on board (Bloor, Thomas, & Lane, 2000). With increased communication technology available, the radio operator became obsolete. This has led to an


increase of work tasks and new demands of


knowledge for those remaining on board. The role of communication is naturally important, both between ship- ship and ship-shore but also, and less frequently supported, on board an individual ship. Nevertheless


it has been


known for some time that communication on board can affect safety and well-being, as seen in studies of for example safety culture (Ek, 2006); effects of multi-lingual crews (Sampson & Zhao, 2003) and the effects of communication on stress levels (Koester, 2007).


Much of the maritime communication research has been focused on finding a common language to communicate (usually “maritime English”, see e.g. (Hughes, 2000)), but less effort


is put


communication. The type of communication and the media used are more or less suited to


into finding methods or tools varying types


for of


communication. For instance, the media richness theory can be applied to communication (Daft & Lengel, 1986), in which the capacity to process rich information is analysed; the richer the medium, the more information it can process. Which medium to choose depends on the need to resolve equivocality (ambiguity, multiple or conflicting interpretations) and uncertainty. In order of increasing richness, we have (including media that were not available in 1986):


 Posters, brochures  Letters  E-mail  Video e-mail  Telephone  Video conference  Face-to-face meetings


The medium to choose also depends, for instance, on context, the need for feedback and cues, the time a piece of C-12 ©2013: The Royal Institution of Naval Architects


not only from an interaction point of view, but also regarding cost-efficiency concerns. If we buy and install innovative systems in our ships, we would probably like them to be used to the fullest potential, which is not always the case (Grabowski, 1989).


4. COMMUNICATION


Page 1  |  Page 2  |  Page 3  |  Page 4  |  Page 5  |  Page 6  |  Page 7  |  Page 8  |  Page 9  |  Page 10  |  Page 11  |  Page 12  |  Page 13  |  Page 14  |  Page 15  |  Page 16  |  Page 17  |  Page 18  |  Page 19  |  Page 20  |  Page 21  |  Page 22  |  Page 23  |  Page 24  |  Page 25  |  Page 26  |  Page 27  |  Page 28  |  Page 29  |  Page 30  |  Page 31  |  Page 32  |  Page 33  |  Page 34  |  Page 35  |  Page 36  |  Page 37  |  Page 38  |  Page 39  |  Page 40  |  Page 41  |  Page 42  |  Page 43  |  Page 44  |  Page 45  |  Page 46  |  Page 47  |  Page 48  |  Page 49  |  Page 50  |  Page 51  |  Page 52  |  Page 53  |  Page 54  |  Page 55  |  Page 56  |  Page 57  |  Page 58  |  Page 59  |  Page 60  |  Page 61  |  Page 62  |  Page 63  |  Page 64  |  Page 65  |  Page 66  |  Page 67  |  Page 68  |  Page 69  |  Page 70  |  Page 71  |  Page 72  |  Page 73  |  Page 74  |  Page 75  |  Page 76  |  Page 77  |  Page 78  |  Page 79  |  Page 80  |  Page 81  |  Page 82  |  Page 83  |  Page 84  |  Page 85  |  Page 86  |  Page 87  |  Page 88  |  Page 89  |  Page 90  |  Page 91  |  Page 92  |  Page 93  |  Page 94  |  Page 95  |  Page 96  |  Page 97  |  Page 98