Trans RINA, Vol 155, Part C1, Intl J Marine Design, Jan -Jun 2013 information/knowledge relating to the task and the
environment (held either by individuals or captured and processed by devices) changes as the situation develops (Stewart, Stanton, Harris, Baber, Salmon, Mock, Tatlock, Wells and Kay, 2007).
Note that the data that underlies DSA is dispersed across human and non-human components in the system. There is also implicit communication of information rather than a detailed exchange of mental models.
In this case the
agents (human and technological) were co-located, however in a larger ship there is no reason why this should always be so. The agents (may be separated by some distance.
There are six basic propositions underpinning DSA (Stanton, Stewart, Baber, Harris, Houghton, McMaster, Salmon, Hoyle, Walker, Young, Linsell and Dymott, 2006).
SA can be held by both human and non-human agents (as depicted in Table 1); technological artefacts and human operators each have some level of SA in as much as they both hold pertinent information pertaining to the situation.
There multiple views on the SA of the same scene for different agents (again, as shown in Table 1): the radar, Navigator and Coxswain all have
situation,
slightly as
different views on illustrated in their
the same perception,
comprehension and projection of the unfolding incident.
Non-overlapping and overlapping SA depends on the agent’s goals: the goal of the radar is to detect objects in the environment; the goal of the Navigator is to determine the level of risk presented by an object; the goal of the Coxswain is to decide on the appropriate response for the crew ensure the safety of the boat.
In terms of
Endsley’s model it could be asserted that the different agents are primarily concerned with representing different stages of SA, rather being
Table 1 Agent
Radar (Non-Human)
Navigator (Human)
Coxswain (Human)
than microcosms of SA in themselves: the radar perceiving;
the Coxswain projecting ahead.
Communication between agents may be in the form of non-verbal behaviour; implicit customs and practice, formal communication channels (as in BRM/MRM) or via interactions with equipment interfaces.
SA holds loosely-coupled, JCSs together: the relationship between the radar, Navigator and Coxswain in the example in Table 1 is held together by the by their respective levels (or stages) of awareness of a potentially dangerous obstacle in the environment and subsequently assessing the most appropriate response.
One agent may compensate for degradation in SA in another agent: for example the Coxswain may be unaware of the
informed by the radar or the other member.
rocks until he is crew
The concept of DSA was subsequently investigated across a much larger team that that utilised in the previous example in the analysis of activities in the operations room of a Type 23 frigate during a variety of simulated engagements. Originally, the Type 23 frigate’s primary task was of anti-submarine warfare but this has been expanded to include air and surface warfare.
All
engagements possessed a number of common discrete steps: plan
resources and strategy; control
external resources (to the operations room and ship); posture platform for attack; identify and classify targets; assess threat and allocate targets; engage targets and re- allocate assets and weapons. For each of these steps the data (or information) requirements and the properties of those data were identified, irrespective of who (or what in terms of sensor systems or equipment) held the data. As engagements progressed, the map of personnel and/or equipment holding
various pieces of information
changed and the communications patterns could also be tracked across the operations room.
DSA as part of a response to unexpected rock formation (taken from Dobbins, Harris, Smoker, Hill, Forsman, Brand, Dahlman and Stark (2010).
Input
Senses rocks ahead of the HSC
Sees rock formation on radar/navigation display
Sees Navigator interrogate radar/navigation display and charts
Process
Compiles picture of extent of rock formation, distance and bearing
Determines rocks may present a risk to the HSC
Determines rocks present a risk to the craft and crew
Output
Displays information (along with projected tack) in appropriate colour to alert crew.
Needs to quickly determine new route to avoid the rock formation
Initiates new route/revision as instructed by the navigator
the Navigator comprehending and
C-6
©2013: The Royal Institution of Naval Architects
Page 1 |
Page 2 |
Page 3 |
Page 4 |
Page 5 |
Page 6 |
Page 7 |
Page 8 |
Page 9 |
Page 10 |
Page 11 |
Page 12 |
Page 13 |
Page 14 |
Page 15 |
Page 16 |
Page 17 |
Page 18 |
Page 19 |
Page 20 |
Page 21 |
Page 22 |
Page 23 |
Page 24 |
Page 25 |
Page 26 |
Page 27 |
Page 28 |
Page 29 |
Page 30 |
Page 31 |
Page 32 |
Page 33 |
Page 34 |
Page 35 |
Page 36 |
Page 37 |
Page 38 |
Page 39 |
Page 40 |
Page 41 |
Page 42 |
Page 43 |
Page 44 |
Page 45 |
Page 46 |
Page 47 |
Page 48 |
Page 49 |
Page 50 |
Page 51 |
Page 52 |
Page 53 |
Page 54 |
Page 55 |
Page 56 |
Page 57 |
Page 58 |
Page 59 |
Page 60 |
Page 61 |
Page 62 |
Page 63 |
Page 64 |
Page 65 |
Page 66 |
Page 67 |
Page 68 |
Page 69 |
Page 70 |
Page 71 |
Page 72 |
Page 73 |
Page 74 |
Page 75 |
Page 76 |
Page 77 |
Page 78 |
Page 79 |
Page 80 |
Page 81 |
Page 82 |
Page 83 |
Page 84 |
Page 85 |
Page 86 |
Page 87 |
Page 88 |
Page 89 |
Page 90 |
Page 91 |
Page 92 |
Page 93 |
Page 94 |
Page 95 |
Page 96 |
Page 97 |
Page 98