search.noResults

search.searching

saml.title
dataCollection.invalidEmail
note.createNoteMessage

search.noResults

search.searching

orderForm.title

orderForm.productCode
orderForm.description
orderForm.quantity
orderForm.itemPrice
orderForm.price
orderForm.totalPrice
orderForm.deliveryDetails.billingAddress
orderForm.deliveryDetails.deliveryAddress
orderForm.noItems
780 L. Von Hagen et al.


tolerance for coexisting with wildlife (Naughton-Treves & Treves, 2005; Kansky & Knight, 2014). Traditionalmethods to deter elephants include patrolling,


guarding (Killion et al., 2020), lighting fires, erecting fences constructed from locally available materials (Kassilly et al., 2008; Osipova et al., 2018) and using devices to scare elephants (Gilsdorf et al., 2002). However, these often achieve only limited success as elephants can habituate to sounds or the presence of humans, and learn to overcome fences (Hoare, 2012; Mumby & Plotnik, 2018; Gross, 2019). Information on deterrent methods is often provided by NGOs or wildlife agencies, which usually operate with lim- ited funding and resources (Folke et al., 2005; Noga et al., 2016; Galvin et al., 2018). Additionally, such traditional ap- proaches often require human presence to deter elephants, which can be dangerous for farmers. Modern deterrents can be more difficult to implement


because the required materials are often not available locally and/or are expensive to purchase (Vogel et al., 2022). How- ever, methods using electric fencing, chili (Parker&Osborn, 2006; Hedges & Gunaryadi, 2010; Chang’a et al., 2016), beehives (King, 2010), solar lights (Adams et al., 2020) and metal fencing (Von Hagen et al., 2021) may achieve greater efficacy as they involve recurring negative stimuli. Furthermore, such methods require no human presence when elephants are present, making them safer and freeing up farmers to fulfil their other duties. Successful designs for deterrents consider elephant physiology, behaviour and cognition (Mumby & Plotnik, 2018), and the socio- economic limitations of farmers, to create mitigation techniques that are also resistant to elephant habituation (Naughton-Treves & Treves, 2005; Dickman, 2010;Schulte, 2016).


The attitudes and behaviours of farmers towards ele-


phants, conservation initiatives and the uptake and use of elephant deterrents have been examined in several regions in Africa (Graham & Ochieng, 2008; Noga et al., 2015; Vogel et al., 2022). However, the socio-economic factors that determine whether or not farmers decide to implement deterrents have not been fully explored. Ageing tends to make people more risk averse (Okun & Siegler, 1976; Dohmen et al., 2011), yet decision-making is often affected by local culture and can be highly variable (Rieger & Mata, 2015). Thus, older farmers may be hesitant to try new techniques or crop types but have often accumulated significant local knowledge of elephant movements and behaviours (Buchholtz et al., 2020). Education levels are highly variable amongst rural farmers (Noga et al., 2015), and higher education levels can positively affect farmer productivity and the adoption of new farming techniques (Oduro-Ofori, 2014). Understanding the factors that affect farming decisions is important to improve elephant conser- vation and management efforts, and provide benefits to farmers.


Given the gaps in knowledge about farmer decision-


making regarding deterrent implementation, the threats to livelihoods of rural farmers in Kenya and the need to con- serve threatened African elephants, our goal was to examine the relationships between socio-economic factors and crop damage by elephants. We developed three a priori hypoth- eses: Firstly, we hypothesized that age, education, exposure to information on deterrents and farm size would be posi- tively associated with deterrent usage by farmers and that most would use traditional methods (deterrent-use hypoth- esis). Secondly, we hypothesized that most rural farming households had not been exposed to information on miti- gating the impacts of crop foraging but that, amongst those who had, education would be positively correlated with receipt of any such information, particularly on fencing (deterrent-exposure hypothesis). Thirdly, we hypothesized that most farmers who believed that they could not imple- ment deterrents (even if they had the relevant knowledge) would be constrained by limited economic resources and that only education level would be positively correlated with farmers who believed that they could implement deter- rents (economic-barriers hypothesis). The evaluation of these hypotheses will inform conservation planning strat- egies for agencies aiming to improve food security for farm- ers whilst conserving elephants.


Study area


The Kasigau Wildlife Corridor lies between Tsavo East and West National Parks in south-eastern Kenya (Fig. 1) and forms part of the Greater Tsavo Ecosystem. African savan- nah elephants Loxodonta africana are frequent crop fora- gers in this area, as are other herbivores such as the eland Tragelaphus oryx, and they are a source of conflict between community members and wildlife officials (Litoroh et al., 2012; Githiru et al., 2017). The region is home to the largest elephant population in Kenya, consisting of c. 15,000 indi- viduals (Waweru et al., 2021), and many elephants use the Corridor to move between the two national parks (Omondi et al., 2008; Ngene et al., 2017). Rukinga Wildlife Sanctuary (Rukinga) is one of the community ranches in the Corridor and is operated by Wildlife Works. Villagers in these areas are mostly subsistence farmers, and their income is c. KSH 1,500 (USD 15) per person per month. In years with lower crop yields 39% of the population drops below this income level. The area is characterized by a biannual rainfall pattern of rainy and dry seasons, and has been suffering from ongoing periods of drought (Kasaine & Githiru, 2016). We selected villages surrounding the Sanctuary to test


our hypotheses, focusing on those that shared a boundary with the Sanctuary (where interactions occur often), were within 1 h drive of the centralized base in Rukinga (for


Oryx, 2024, 58(6), 779–787 © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Fauna & Flora International doi:10.1017/S0030605323001795


Page 1  |  Page 2  |  Page 3  |  Page 4  |  Page 5  |  Page 6  |  Page 7  |  Page 8  |  Page 9  |  Page 10  |  Page 11  |  Page 12  |  Page 13  |  Page 14  |  Page 15  |  Page 16  |  Page 17  |  Page 18  |  Page 19  |  Page 20  |  Page 21  |  Page 22  |  Page 23  |  Page 24  |  Page 25  |  Page 26  |  Page 27  |  Page 28  |  Page 29  |  Page 30  |  Page 31  |  Page 32  |  Page 33  |  Page 34  |  Page 35  |  Page 36  |  Page 37  |  Page 38  |  Page 39  |  Page 40  |  Page 41  |  Page 42  |  Page 43  |  Page 44  |  Page 45  |  Page 46  |  Page 47  |  Page 48  |  Page 49  |  Page 50  |  Page 51  |  Page 52  |  Page 53  |  Page 54  |  Page 55  |  Page 56  |  Page 57  |  Page 58  |  Page 59  |  Page 60  |  Page 61  |  Page 62  |  Page 63  |  Page 64  |  Page 65  |  Page 66  |  Page 67  |  Page 68  |  Page 69  |  Page 70  |  Page 71  |  Page 72  |  Page 73  |  Page 74  |  Page 75  |  Page 76  |  Page 77  |  Page 78  |  Page 79  |  Page 80  |  Page 81  |  Page 82  |  Page 83  |  Page 84  |  Page 85  |  Page 86  |  Page 87  |  Page 88  |  Page 89  |  Page 90  |  Page 91  |  Page 92  |  Page 93  |  Page 94  |  Page 95  |  Page 96  |  Page 97  |  Page 98  |  Page 99  |  Page 100  |  Page 101  |  Page 102  |  Page 103  |  Page 104  |  Page 105  |  Page 106  |  Page 107  |  Page 108  |  Page 109  |  Page 110  |  Page 111  |  Page 112  |  Page 113  |  Page 114  |  Page 115  |  Page 116  |  Page 117  |  Page 118  |  Page 119  |  Page 120  |  Page 121  |  Page 122  |  Page 123  |  Page 124  |  Page 125  |  Page 126  |  Page 127  |  Page 128  |  Page 129  |  Page 130  |  Page 131  |  Page 132  |  Page 133  |  Page 134  |  Page 135  |  Page 136  |  Page 137  |  Page 138  |  Page 139  |  Page 140