Infection Control & Hospital Epidemiology (2019), 40,89–94 doi:10.1017/ice.2018.277
Original Article
Testing a novel audit and feedback method for hand hygiene compliance: A multicenter quality improvement study
Aaron M. Scherer PhD1, Heather Schacht Reisinger PhD1,2, Michihiko Goto MD, MSCI1,2, Cassie Cunningham Goedken MPH2, Gosia S. Clore MPH1, Alexandre R. Marra MD1,2,3, Emily E. Chasco PhD2,
Charlesnika T. Evans PhD, MPH4,5, Michael A. Rubin MD, PhD6,7 and Eli N. Perencevich MD, MPH1,2 1Department of Internal Medicine, University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa, United States, 2Center for Access & Delivery Research and Evaluation (CADRE), Iowa City Veterans Affairs Health Care System, Iowa City, Iowa, United States, 3Division of Medical Practice, Hospital Israelita Albert Einstein, São Paulo, Brazil, 4Center of Innovation for Complex Chronic Healthcare (CINCCH), Edward Hines Jr Veterans Affairs Hospital, Chicago, Illinois, United States, 5Center for Healthcare Studies and Department of Preventive Medicine Institute for Public Health and Medicine, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, United States, 6Salt Lake Veterans Affairs Informatics, Decision Enhancement, and Surveillance (IDEAS) Center, Salt Lake City, Utah, United States and 7Department of Internal Medicine, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, United States
Abstract
Objective: Although most hospitals report very high levels of hand hygiene compliance (HHC), the accuracy of these overtly observed rates is questionable due to the Hawthorne effect and other sources of bias. In the study, we aimed (1) to compare HHC rates estimated using the standard audit method of overt observation by a known observer and a new audit method that employed a rapid (<15 minutes) “secret shopper” method and (2) to pilot test a novel feedback tool. Design: Quality improvement project using a quasi-experimental stepped-wedge design. Setting: This study was conducted in 5 acute-care hospitals (17 wards, 5 intensive care units) in the Midwestern United States. Methods: Sites recruited a hand hygiene observer from outside the acute-care units to rapidly and covertly observe entry and exit HHC during the study period, October 2016–September 2017. After 3 months of observations, sites received a monthly feedback tool that communicated HHC information from the new audit method. Results: The absolute difference in HHC estimates between the standard and new audit methods was ~30%. No significant differences in HHC were detected between the baseline and feedback phases (OR, 0.92; 95% CI, 0.84–1.01), but the standard audit method had significantly higher estimates than the new audit method (OR, 9.83; 95% CI, 8.82–10.95). Conclusions: HHC estimates obtained using the new audit method were substantially lower than estimates obtained using the standard audit method, suggesting that the rapid, secret-shopper method is less subject to bias. Providing feedback using HHC from the new audit method did not seem to impact HHC behaviors.
(Received 27 June 2018; accepted 6 October 2018; electronically published 15 November 2018)
Despite the critical importance of hand hygiene in the prevention of healthcare-associated infections (HAIs), the extent to which healthcare workers engage in appropriate levels of hand hygiene is unclear. The standard audit method of overtly measuring hand hygiene compliance (HHC) is subject to observer biases, and many hospitals report HHC rates that are misleadingly high. For example, in 2009 The Joint Commission led a quality improve- ment study targeting HHC, in which facilities reported com- pliance as high as 85%, rather than the 48% measured using more accurate methods.1 One reason that data collected using standard audit methods might overestimate compliance is that audits are frequently conducted during a scheduled time by an auditor that
Author for correspondence: Aaron M. Scherer, Department of Internal Medicine,
University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Dr, Iowa City, IA 52241. E-mail: aaron-scherer@uiowa. edu
Cite this article: Scherer AM, et al. (2019) Testing a novel audit and feedback method
for hand hygiene compliance: A multicenter quality improvement study. Infection Control & Hospital Epidemiology 2019, 40, 89–94. doi: 10.1017/ice.2018.277
is known by the unit staff. Thus, HHC rates observed using standard audit methods may be susceptible to bias from the Hawthorne effect.2–6 The Hawthorne effect is a phenomenon in which people engage in higher levels of socially desirable behavior when they feel like they are being observed. The consequence of the Hawthorne effect for hand hygiene is that any HHC mea- surement that occurs when individuals are actively aware of being observed will likely lead to higher HHC estimates.7–13 Consistent with this prediction, a recent review of studies highlighted that HHC rates are significantly lower when observations are unannounced, short (<15 minutes), and conducted by “secret shoppers”: individuals hired to covertly observe HHC by surreptitiously appearing as a member of the public or as researchers studying a topic unrelated to HHC.2 This rapid (<15-minute) benchmark using covert observations
was first established in a study at 5 ICUs and 6 medical/surgical wards.13 A “secret shopper” sat and “read” in a hallway, recording entry and exit HHC covertly for 1 hour. The Hawthorne effect
This work is classified, for copyright purposes, as a work of the U.S. Government and is not subject to copyright protection within the United States.
Page 1 |
Page 2 |
Page 3 |
Page 4 |
Page 5 |
Page 6 |
Page 7 |
Page 8 |
Page 9 |
Page 10 |
Page 11 |
Page 12 |
Page 13 |
Page 14 |
Page 15 |
Page 16 |
Page 17 |
Page 18 |
Page 19 |
Page 20 |
Page 21 |
Page 22 |
Page 23 |
Page 24 |
Page 25 |
Page 26 |
Page 27 |
Page 28 |
Page 29 |
Page 30 |
Page 31 |
Page 32 |
Page 33 |
Page 34 |
Page 35 |
Page 36 |
Page 37 |
Page 38 |
Page 39 |
Page 40 |
Page 41 |
Page 42 |
Page 43 |
Page 44 |
Page 45 |
Page 46 |
Page 47 |
Page 48 |
Page 49 |
Page 50 |
Page 51 |
Page 52 |
Page 53 |
Page 54 |
Page 55 |
Page 56 |
Page 57 |
Page 58 |
Page 59 |
Page 60 |
Page 61 |
Page 62 |
Page 63 |
Page 64 |
Page 65 |
Page 66 |
Page 67 |
Page 68 |
Page 69 |
Page 70 |
Page 71 |
Page 72 |
Page 73 |
Page 74 |
Page 75 |
Page 76 |
Page 77 |
Page 78 |
Page 79 |
Page 80 |
Page 81 |
Page 82 |
Page 83 |
Page 84 |
Page 85 |
Page 86 |
Page 87 |
Page 88 |
Page 89 |
Page 90 |
Page 91 |
Page 92 |
Page 93 |
Page 94 |
Page 95 |
Page 96 |
Page 97 |
Page 98 |
Page 99 |
Page 100 |
Page 101 |
Page 102 |
Page 103 |
Page 104 |
Page 105 |
Page 106 |
Page 107 |
Page 108 |
Page 109 |
Page 110 |
Page 111 |
Page 112 |
Page 113 |
Page 114 |
Page 115 |
Page 116 |
Page 117 |
Page 118 |
Page 119 |
Page 120 |
Page 121 |
Page 122 |
Page 123 |
Page 124 |
Page 125 |
Page 126 |
Page 127 |
Page 128 |
Page 129 |
Page 130 |
Page 131 |
Page 132 |
Page 133 |
Page 134 |
Page 135 |
Page 136