This page contains a Flash digital edition of a book.
MicroscopyEducation


Figure 8 : Who’s looking at whom? Two nested loupes give 10× views.


Dusty Miller and compare it to fur. She might ask herself: “If the surface of this leaf reminds me of fur, I wonder if it might function or act like a kind of fur for the survival of the plant? How could it help the leaf/plant survive to have a kind of fur? And how would I test that?” Increased magnification . The magnification journey begins at 5×; but two loupes may be nested for 10× ( Figure 8 ). Fresh details emerge. Students delight in the shift from 5× to 10× and begin to experience what scale means, both physically and conceptually. If there are hidden wonders at 5× and 10×, think what awaits at 50×, 100×, and10,000×. As students incrementally change the scale, they make new analogies, becoming more and more intimate with the world—the first step to falling in love with the world. Note that a 5× loupe makes the world strange, but not too strange. Beginning the journey at 50× or 100× is simply too abstract for most students, even in high school and college. But starting close to human scale, adding person- alized metaphors and similes along the way, it is easy to relate to the world fifty times bigger in the compound lab microscope. Continue this habit and students can leap to an interest in worlds thousands of times larger. Scanning electron microscopy (SEM) of loupe-viewed objects helps students extend the journey, which is why a gallery of SEM images by microscopist Dennis Kunkel is included in the book The Private Eye — (5 × ) Looking/Thinking by Analogy . Writing . Back in the classroom, the fi ſt h-graders from Cascade Locks elaborate on their lists. Each student writes a “sandwich poem,” an easy form where the title and the last line are the same. T e middle of “the sandwich” is fi lled with favorite lines from a loupe-list. One girl hunches over her notebook and writes:


Fern A green fountain. T e leaves are swords, the edges like teeth. T ere are hairs like rusty wires. And on the back, armies of tiny buttons. Fern


The students will use their starter loupe-list for extended nature journaling, for investigative essaying, or for setting


2015 May • www.microscopy-today.com


Figure 9 : Loupe-drawing develops attention to detail, pattern sensitivity, and a sense of scale, even for those who think they cannot draw.


up hypotheses. What are those “buttons” on the back? Why are they in rows? What do they do? Why do the tiny parts of the leaves look the same as the whole leaf? How can we test our ideas?


Drawing . Soon the students begin to loupe-draw a leaf, a fallen pinecone, or a mossy twig. T ey move between loupe- studying the object and drawing a section of the object. It’s the same with fi rst-graders, high schoolers, or adults—the drawing process is absorbing. T e loupe gives such intense visual feedback that even those who think they can’t draw discover they can ( Figure 9 )! T e activity develops pattern sensitivity, attention to detail, and a personal sense of what scale means ( Figure 10 ). Furthermore, education research strongly supports the role of non-linguistic representation as a necessary component of conceptual development.


Research on Learning T e basis of T e Private Eye’s instructional strategy was already in place in the 1960s and 1970s (for example, Piaget [ 5 ]; Brownowski [ 6 ]). T is early work has been underscored by more recent neurocognitive and educational research, including the work of cognitive scientist Douglas Hofstadter, who broadly makes the case for analogy as “the core of cognition” [ 7 ]. When teachers and students move through T e Private Eye’s steps for writing, drawing, and hypothesizing, they engage in each of the nine most eff ective instructional strategies for raising student achievement noted by Marzano et al. [ 8 ]. Two examples of these nine strategies are: (a) identifying similarities and diff erences and (b) nonlinguistic representation.


Similarities and diff erences . If students develop a habit of identifying similarities and diff erences, achievement rises by a whopping 45% [ 8 ]. Students build this habit as they write their analogy lists while loupe-studying an object, as they repeatedly notice and discuss similarities and diff erences between an object and whatever else it reminds them of. T ere is even some recent work that points to identifying similarities as the subset of abstract reasoning accounting for the rise in IQ scores noted over the years [ 9 ].


55


Page 1  |  Page 2  |  Page 3  |  Page 4  |  Page 5  |  Page 6  |  Page 7  |  Page 8  |  Page 9  |  Page 10  |  Page 11  |  Page 12  |  Page 13  |  Page 14  |  Page 15  |  Page 16  |  Page 17  |  Page 18  |  Page 19  |  Page 20  |  Page 21  |  Page 22  |  Page 23  |  Page 24  |  Page 25  |  Page 26  |  Page 27  |  Page 28  |  Page 29  |  Page 30  |  Page 31  |  Page 32  |  Page 33  |  Page 34  |  Page 35  |  Page 36  |  Page 37  |  Page 38  |  Page 39  |  Page 40  |  Page 41  |  Page 42  |  Page 43  |  Page 44  |  Page 45  |  Page 46  |  Page 47  |  Page 48  |  Page 49  |  Page 50  |  Page 51  |  Page 52  |  Page 53  |  Page 54  |  Page 55  |  Page 56  |  Page 57  |  Page 58  |  Page 59  |  Page 60  |  Page 61  |  Page 62  |  Page 63  |  Page 64  |  Page 65  |  Page 66  |  Page 67  |  Page 68  |  Page 69  |  Page 70  |  Page 71  |  Page 72  |  Page 73  |  Page 74  |  Page 75  |  Page 76